Health

New Study Shows Infants Have a Surprising Memory Capacity That Lasts Longer Than We Thought!

2025-03-20

Author: Yu

Introduction

In a groundbreaking study published in Science, researchers have shattered long-held beliefs about infant memory, uncovering that babies have the ability to form memories far earlier than we assumed. This research sheds light on a phenomenon known as infantile amnesia—the inability to recall experiences from early childhood.

Key Insights from the Study

Nick Turk-Browne, a psychology professor at Yale and the lead author of the study, expressed his fascination with the mysteries surrounding early memory development. “Our earliest years are filled with learning milestones, yet we typically can’t recall them—there’s a puzzling disconnect between our incredible ability to learn and the lack of memories,” he told AFP.

Children begin to exhibit extraordinary learning abilities around the age of one, which includes mastering language, walking, and forming social connections. Despite this, our memories from that time period seem to vanish, previously leading to various theories including one proposed by Sigmund Freud that suggested childhood memories are repressed. However, modern science largely attributes memory loss to the hippocampus, a brain region essential for episodic memory that isn’t fully matured in infants.

Research Findings

Intriguingly, earlier behavioral research hinted at the existence of memory even in the youngest of children. Since babies cannot verbally articulate their memories until they learn to speak, their longer gaze at familiar objects serves as a strong indicator of their recall abilities.

Research involving rodents has shown that memories are indeed formed in the infant hippocampus, but the pathways to access these memories might fade as they grow older. Techniques involving light stimulation to reawaken these memories have shown promise, hinting at the complexity of memory retention in early development.

Challenges in Infants' Brain Imaging Studies

However, conducting brain imaging studies with infants has posed a significant challenge—infants are notoriously squirmy and uncooperative in functional MRI (fMRI) machines. To tackle this issue, Turk-Browne's team employed innovative strategies: using familiar items like pacifiers and stuffed animals to keep babies calm, padding them with pillows, and even creating psychedelic background images to hold their attention.

Memorization Tasks and Observations

Despite the challenges, including inevitable movement leading to blurry images, the research team conducted hundreds of sessions, involving 26 infants—half under one year and half over one year old. They adapted a memory task typically used in adult studies, showing infants images of faces, scenes, or objects, and later testing their memory by presenting previously seen images alongside new ones.

The researchers measured the baby’s memory by observing how long they looked at familiar images. What they found provided exciting evidence that the hippocampus is indeed active in encoding memories as early as one year of age. Notably, this was confirmed for 11 of the 13 infants over one year, while infants under one showed less hippocampal activity during memory formation.

Conclusions and Future Directions

“In conclusion, we have shown that infants have the ability to encode episodic memories in the hippocampus starting around the age of one,” Turk-Browne stated, raising the question: What happens to these early memories as we grow?

An editorial from researchers Adam Ramsaran and Paul Frankland acknowledged the ingenuity of the study's design while emphasizing that the fate of these early memories remains uncertain. Are they never fully stored, or do they persist but become inaccessible over time? Turk-Browne suggests the latter and is now spearheading a follow-up study exploring whether children can recognize video clips recorded from their own perspective when they were younger.

Preliminary findings are tantalizing; they hint that early memories may last until approximately age three before they fade from conscious access. Turk-Browne is particularly intrigued by the notion that these memories could potentially be reactivated later in life, opening a new chapter in our understanding of memory retention in early childhood.

Conclusion

As the researchers continue to dive deeper into these questions, we may soon uncover the secrets of how our earliest memories shape who we are. Stay tuned for what this exciting research reveals next!